"Call it a Clan, call it a Network, call it a Tribe, call it a Family. Whatever you call it, whoever you are, you need one." - Jane Howard .
Wednesday, November 7, 2007
VERY interesting news letter
Newsletter Of The China Adoption Research Program, NOV. 2007 PAGE 1
NOVEMBER, 2007
University of South Florida, 4202 E. Fowler Ave., EDU162, Tampa, FL 33620-8335
Tel: 813-974-6496 ● Fax: 813-974-5814 ●
CHINA ADOPTION RESEARCH PROGRAM
Mission: The China Adoption Research Program at the University of South Florida
seeks to conduct rigorous longitudinal developmental research on child and family
outcomes associated with the adoption of children from China, and to use the
findings of such research to inform the adopted children’s teachers, active and
prospective adoptive families, as well as policy makers, managers, consultants,
support groups, and agencies involved in the adoption process.
Director & Research Team Leader:
Dr. Tony Xing Tan
tan@coedu.usf.edu
Psychological and Social Foundations
Tel: 813-974-6496 ● Fax: 813-974-5814
Members of the Research Team:
Robert F. Dedrick, Ph.D.
Educational Measurement & Research
Kofi Marfo, Ph.D.
CRCDL & Psychological and Social
Foundations
MESSAGE FROM DR. TAM
Dear Parents:
I hope you are doing well.
In this letter, I will be sharing with you my meeting with the
CCAA officials on August 14th of this year, and some preliminary
results on parents’ report of their children’s development.
Thank you very much for helping me with the research for
the second time. I was able to gather data from 670 families (out
of the 853 families in the 2005 phase of the study). Unfortunately,
there were about 120 families who participated in the 2005
study but their contact information was no longer valid. I am
hoping that they will get in touch with me after reading this
newsletter.
The 2007 study is designed to learn about the adopted children’s
development since 2 years ago. In this phase of the study, I
focused on preschool children’s sleep patterns and sleep problems.
For the school-aged children, I focused on their social
skills and academic skills. I asked the parents to fill out a blue
form (the same as the one they filled out in 2005) called the
Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL). I also asked them to fill out a
Social Skill Rating System (SSRS) and Sleep and Parenting Survey
(for preschool children only). In this phase I also asked parents
if they would be willing to ask their adopted children’s
teacher to fill out a survey. I am sending the parents the surveys
for the teachers this week.
I have again arranged the findings in a Q & A format so that
you can choose to read the ones that interest you. I hope you
find them informative.
Newsletter Of The China Adoption Research Program, NOV. 2007 PAGE 2
MEETING WITH CCAA AND PRELIMINARY FINDINGS FROM THE 2007 STUDY
On August 14th, I met with the CCAA officials in
their new building. While there, I noticed that the
CCAA was busy welcoming children in Beijing for
heritage tours. When I was at the office of the deputy
director Ms. Chu XiaoYing (who was the Director of
Document Review office in 2002 when I last visited),
I noticed a large poster with many photos of the
adopted children (current photo, current residence,
adoption photo, orphanage, as well as a short message
the child wrote to CCAA). I believe a couple of
the children are actually in my study.
At the CCAA, I had individual meetings with Ms.
Chu and Ms. Tie Ling (Director of Infant and Toddler
Care). I also had a brief meeting with Mr. Ji (Director
of Domestic Adoption) (who was Director of
the Matching Office in 2002 when I last visited). I
did not get the opportunity to meet with Mr. Lu (Director
of CCAA). Hopefully, when I visit again, he
will have time to meet.
My meeting with Ms. Chu focused on the current
CCAA policy. We discussed some of the policy
changes that had evoked strong reactions from the
adoptive parents. The discussions were open and Ms
Chu offered her insights that might help us better understand
the policy change. The summary of the discussions
are as follows:
1. Why did CCAA change the adoption policy given
that it has been working well?
According to Ms. Chu there were no fundamental
changes in the CCAA adoption policy. The current
practice was a reaction to the decrease in the number
of children available for adoption. As the number of
children decreases, it is necessary for CCAA to identify
families that are deemed to be most suited to
raise the orphanage children.
2. Single parents have been shown to be just as
successful in raising adopted Chinese children,
why are they not allowed to adopt any longer?
Again Ms. Chu suggested that it was not because
they did not believe in the single parents’ ability to
raise children. It is because of the limited number of
children available. She also raised questions about
whether adolescence will present more challenges for
the adopted children, especially those from singleparent
households.
3. Why is it taking so much longer than before to
adopt?
Ms. Chu commented that this was occurring because
there were too many applications piled up. The
CCAA must process these applications before they
can get to the newer ones. We talked at length about
how the extended waiting might have affected the
children as well as the parents. I mentioned that extended
waiting might push people away from China
and some parents might experience major life events
while waiting, which might lead to a change of plans
and decisions to adopt (I borrowed these thoughts
from another adoptive mother). Ms. Chu was sympathetic
about this but was unable to offer any better
solutions. I subsequently visited their matching office
again. This time, there were between 12-16 staff
members working (in 2002, there were fewer staff
members working on matching). Some of them have
their cubic walls covered with baby headshots. I
asked Ms. Chu about matching and she said it was
more like an intuition than science. The staff members
typically read through the applications to gain
some sense of whom the adoptive parents are and
based on that, they try to find a child that they believe
is the best match.
4. Why is that the new policy requires a very high
income? Many families who have adopted and
raised Chinese children would not have been able
to do so if the policy was in place at that time.
Ms. Chu said that it was not true that they required an
income of about $80,000. She said they wanted the
potential adoptive parents to show evidence of assets
of at least such a value. She commented that it would
be much easier for a family to qualify if they own
their house/apartment.
5. Will the policy change back after the Olympics?
Newsletter Of The China Adoption Research Program, NOV. 2007 PAGE 3
Ms. Chu said that the Olympics did not have anything
to do with the policy change. I suspect that the
CCAA is not responsible for policy-making/change.
It is likely that the Ministry of Civil Affairs plays a
central part in policy issues. As evidence, while in
China I visited an orphanage and was somewhat surprised
to find out that among the staff members greeting
me was the Director of Civil Affairs from the local
city. She was clearly the person “in charge” from
what I could tell.
6. Will the adopted children one day have easier
ways to locate their birth parents?
Ms. Chu felt that the current reality of child abandonment
makes it very difficult to search. She said
usually they would tell the adoptive parents that it is
not likely that the birthparents will be located. She
was very curious about the recent case of a child in
the Netherlands who found and reunited with her
birth family (parents and two older sisters), who live
in a remote part of Chongqing.
7. Has there been an increase in post-adoption
disruption?
Ms. Chu said that she was not aware of the increase.
She mentioned that while the CCAA was sad to hear
that some of the adoptions did not work out, they
were unable to do much once the adopted family left
China.
8. Is it possible that China will close international
adoption if the number of children continues to
decrease?
She did not believe that this would happen any time
soon. She cited South Korea as an example. She suggested
that more children out of wedlock would likely
become a source of international adoption.
Meeting with Ms. Tie
As I am very interested in using research to inform
child care in the orphanages, I scheduled a meeting
with Ms. Tie, Director of the Infant and Toddler Care
Program. In addition to talking about the current policy
change (her response was similar to that of Ms.
Chu), I focused on what was needed to improve the
child-caring quality in the orphanages. Ms. Tie mentioned
that the CCAA was very interested in finding
effective ways to identify high-risk children so that
early interventions could be implemented in a timely
manner. She felt that some systematic applied research
on these children while they are in the orphanages
would be very useful. Additionally, Ms. Tie
also mentioned that the CCAA was interested in finding
better ways to identify potential adoptive parents
for older children. I told Ms. Tie that as a researcher,
I would be happy to take on the task of developing
usable tools that could be used to identify high-risk
children, to help train staff members, and to help
identify potential parents for older children. The discussions
are still at a very preliminary stage. There
are several things to consider very carefully. For instance,
is there a way to ensure that children who are
identified as high-risk indeed receive intended early
interventions (instead of being removed from the list
of potential adoption)? Additionally, Ms. Tie made it
very clear that copyright of any research results
would belong to the CCAA. This makes it hard for
researchers who might be interested in conducting
research in the orphanages. Of course, funding is also
very big concern as the CCAA would not be willing
to sponsor any of the research initiatives. I am currently
brainstorming and conversing with other researchers
regarding this issue. It might be possible to
work with other organizations to test some of the
ideas first.
Brief Meeting with Mr. Ji
Finally my brief meeting with Mr. Ji Gang, Director
of Domestic Adoption, taught me something quite
interesting. He told me that about 20,000 to 30,000
children were adopted domestically a year (I assume
he was talking about recent statistics).
Overall, I found the CCAA to be very interested in
learning about the children, their adoptive families,
their schooling, and their overall life experiences as
adopted individuals. They are eager to find out
whether over the years there is any improvement in
the children’s developmental status at the time of
adoption. I am currently compiling data on the percentage
of children with delays by year of adoption
(e.g., 2004 versus 2000) and by provinces. I will use
Newsletter Of The China Adoption Research Program, NOV. 2007 PAGE 4
the data that I collected while at Harvard in 2002 and
the data that I collected in 2005 and 2007. Hopefully,
this will give the CCAA a better picture of the conditions
of children at adoption over the decade.
They are very excited to hear that some of the children
have done exceptionally well (e.g., in 2000 One
child was featured by the Australia Post on a special
sheetlet of 25 stamps) (I recently learned that the
same child, now in high school, has just won a writing
award and is now one of six finalists at the national
level). They want to know where these high
achievers were adopted from and what their adoptive
parents were like etc. On the day that we met, Ms.
Chu was actually getting ready to host an 11-year old
boy in her home. She was very excited and wanted to
make sure that the child would enjoy the experience.
She mentioned that she wanted to do some “American-
like” things at home so the child would feel more
comfortable. I suggested that it would be just fine for
her to conduct her life as she usually would, so that
the child could learn about Chinese culture.
Preliminary Research Findings
In my meetings with Ms. Chu and Ms. Tie, I also
shared with them some information about the research
that I have conducted. In addition to sharing
with them that the Chinese children showed favorable
post-adoption development, I also discussed a few
findings that seemed to indicate elevated risks (e.g.,
observed signs of neglect at the time of adoption, developmental
delays at the time of adoption, sleep
problems, learning disabilities, and identity development
issues). I believe some of these findings are also
of great interest to the adoptive parents. Below is a
summary of these findings (Some findings were from
the 2005 Data).
1. Observed signs of early neglect and later development.
In 2005, parents were asked to report whether they
observed any of the listed 11 signs (e.g., scars, rashes)
on the children on the day of adoption. We only
asked parents to check on signs that could be easily
observed. Data analysis showed that more signs were
associated with more adjustment problems in the
children. One of the ways that I interpret the results is
that these signs reflect, to some degree, quality of
care that the adopted children received while they
were in orphanages. More signs of early neglect reflect
poorer quality of early experience, which might
have presented more challenges in these children’s
post-adoption development. I have not looked at the
recent data to see if these signs continued to have an
impact on the children’s behavioral development or
social skills. I am hoping that as time goes by, the
impact of the children’s orphanage experience will
gradually diminish. From a policy perspective, this
finding points to the critical need to ensure that adequate
care be provided to orphanage children.
2. Sleep problems among the adopted children.
As a group (total: 410 children), from 2005 to 2007,
preschool-aged adopted children’s sleep problems did
not seem to decrease, despite the fact that many psychologists
have believed that as children get older
they are likely to experience fewer sleep problems (I
did not ask parents about older children’s sleep problems).
In 2005, about 9% of the children had sleep
problems severe enough to be considered clinical or
borderline clinical. In 2007, about 7% of the children
were in this range.
Sleep problems in children seem to have a distinctive
etiology (as compared to other behavior problems).
So far, factors such as age at adoption, current age,
foster care prior to adoption, developmental delays at
adoption and signs of pre-adoption neglect have all
failed to explain the adopted children’s sleep problems.
In the 2007 survey, I asked parents more questions
about their children’s sleep problems/patterns,
their family sleep arrangements, and how they felt
about bed-sharing. The data showed that 43% of the
children require an object (e.g., teddy bear) to sleep,
31% do not want to sleep alone most of the nights,
21% are afraid of sleeping in the dark, about 20%
suck thumb/fingers, 17% frequently come to parents’
bed at night or early in the morning, and about 12%
kick and thrash a lot when asleep. Frequent nightmares
and night terrors only occur in about 2-3% of
the children. Note that it is common for one child to
have several of these behaviors.
In terms of sleep arrangements, about 11% of the
children co-sleep with their parents, around 55% of
Newsletter Of The China Adoption Research Program, NOV. 2007 PAGE 5
the children sleep alone in their own bedroom, and
23% of the children share a bedroom with their siblings.
Single parents are more likely to co-sleep with
their children. For most families, co-sleeping occurred
after the parents had exhausted options in
managing their children’s sleep problems. Overall,
adoptive parents tend to be rather neutral towards cosleeping.
Many parents commented that if it is
needed, they would use it. The most frequently cited
method used to help improve the child’s sleep patterns
is strict/consistent bedtime routines.
Interestingly, I found that very few parents asked
their children’s doctors about co-sleeping. For those
who did ask, the doctors usually discouraged it. Families
who struggle with this issue tend to use the
adoptive community as a resource. When these parents
do ask questions about co-sleeping, they frequently
received mixed messages. The adoptive parents’
extended families also frequently discourage
co-sleeping. This is not surprising as solitary sleeping
is heavily favored in the Western culture. I suspect
that culture preference to solitary sleeping might
make it hard for parents to ask about co-sleeping. I
will be sharing with you more findings on children’s
sleep behaviors soon.
3. Preschool–age adopted children’s behavior
problems over time.
For the preschool-age children (total: 400 children),
from 2005 to 2007 (The average age of the children
was 2.5 years in 2005 and 4.5 years in 2007) their
Internalizing Problems (e.g., anxiety, withdrawn) increased
significantly but there was no significant
change in Externalizing Problems (e.g., aggression). I
am currently looking deeper into the data to find out
more about why there was such a big increase in their
internalizing problems. From 2005 to 2007, the number
of children who were in the clinical/borderline
clinical category increased from 7% to 12% for Internalizing
Problems. For Externalizing Problems, the
number of children in clinical/borderline clinical category
remained 5%. For the normative sample in the
US, 17-20% of children of this age range are in clinical/
borderline clinical category. Thus, the Chinese
children are still better adjusted than the US normative
sample, even if they have more Internalizing
Problems than two years ago.
4. School-age adopted children’s behavior problems
over time.
For the school-age children (total: 276 children),
from 2005 to 2007 (The average age of the children
was 8.8 years in 2005 and 10.6 years in 2007) their
behavior problems did not change much over 2 years.
However, the number of children who fell into clinical/
borderline clinical category increased from 15%
to 20% for Internalizing Problems (e.g., depression,
anxiety) and remained at about 15% for Externalizing
Problems (e.g., aggression). Among the US normative
sample, about 18% of the children in this age
range are in clinical/borderline clinical category. In
other words, the school-aged Chinese children’s behavior
profile is very similar to that of the U.S. normative
sample.
5. Adjustment of children who have crossed over
from preschool age into school age.
From 2005 to 2007, 194 children had “crossed over”
from the preschool group to the school-age group.
For 130 of them, they have entered grade school; for
the rest (64 children), although they are of school age
they have not yet started grade school. For the 130
children who have entered grade school, the number
of children in clinical/borderline clinical category of
Internalizing Problems increased from 10% to 18%;
the number of children in clinical/borderline clinical
category of Externalizing Problems increased from
5% to 12%. For the 64 children who have not started
grade school, the number in clinical/borderline clinical
category in Internalizing Problems increased from
12% to 21%; the number of children in clinical/
borderline clinical category in Externalizing Problems
remained at about 9%.
For the 64 children, I will be asking their parents to
complete the older version of the blue form (i.e., for
children who have started school). Their input will
help me understand whether it is age increase or
school environment/experience that contributed to the
increase in maladjustment in the adopted children.
6. Between Preschool and School-aged children,
why is there a big jump in number of children in
clinical/borderline clinical range?
Newsletter Of The China Adoption Research Program, NOV. 2007 PAGE 6
I have also been very baffled by this finding. Among
the non-adopted children, the number of children in
clinical/borderline clinical category seems rather consistent
(between 18-20%) for both preschool and
school-age children. This is, however, not the case
for the Chinese children. There are several possible
explanations: a). Transitioning to grade school is
more challenging to the adopted Chinese children
than non-adopted children. If this is the case, it is
conceivable that the Chinese children will have a
harder time during school age. However, this speculation
does not explain why these children continue
to show more problems (e.g., after they have completed
the transition) than we would have expected
based on their preschool adjustment. b). School environment,
coupled with the children’s increased understanding
of adoption, makes it more challenging
for the Chinese children. It is possible that as the
children get older and start spending more time without
their parents, they will learn to deal with questions
that many other children would not have to face
(e.g., Why are you adopted? Why do you look different
from your parents?). c). Methodologically, it may
also be possible that the form that I use (the Blue
form that you filled out) might have led to more
children at school-age to score in clinical/borderline
clinical range. The blue form (called the Child Behavior
Checklist) has a preschool version and schoolage
version. Even though the two versions largely
overlap, they do have some differences. However, I
should point out, that these two versions have not
been reported to create discrepancies among children
who are not adopted. I am hoping to talk with some
parents whose children have recently transitioned
from preschool to elementary school, to learn more
about whether the transition tends to be more difficult
for the adopted children.
More importantly, in order to learn how the school
environment might affect the adopted children, I
think a better source of information would be the
children’s teachers. I would like to see if information
provided by the children’s teachers can offer some
insights into their school adjustment. For parents who
have agreed to pass along a survey to their children’s
teacher, I will be sending them a survey soon.
I will be sharing with the research findings periodically.
PUBLISHED AND FORTHCOMING ARTICLES
FROM THE CHINA ADOPTION RESEARCH PROGRAM
Tan, T. X. (2007). History of early neglect and middle
childhood social competence: An adoption
study. Adoption Quarterly, 9 (4), 59-72.
Dedrick, R. F., Tan, T. X. & Marfo, K. (in press).
Factor structure of the child behavior checklist/6-
18 in a sample of girls adopted from China. Psychological
Assessment.
Tan, T. X., Dedrick, R. F. & Marfo, K. (2007). Factor
Structure and Clinical Implications of Child Behavior
Checklist/1½-5 Ratings in a Sample of
Girls Adopted from China. Journal of Pediatric
Psychology, 32, 807-818.
Tan, T. X., Marfo, K. & Dedrick, R. F. (2007). Spe
cial needs adoption from China: Child characteristics
and behavioral adjustment. Children and
Youth Services Review. 29, 1269-1285.
Tan, T. X., & Marfo, K. (2006). Parental ratings of
behavioral adjustment in two samples of adopted
Chinese girls: Age-related versus socio-emotional
correlates and predictors. Journal of Applied Developmental
Psychology, 27(1) 14-30.
Tan, T. X., & Yang, Y. (2005). Language development
of Chinese adoptees 18-35 months. Early
Childhood Research Quarterly, 20, 57-68.
Tan, T. X. (2004). Child outcomes of single-parent
adoption from China: A comparative study.
Adoption Quarterly, 8(1), 1-20.
Tan, T. X. & Nakkula, M. J. (2004). White parents’
attitudes toward their adopted Chinese daughters’
ethnic identity, Adoption Quarterly, 7(4), 57-76.
Subscribe to:
Post Comments (Atom)
No comments:
Post a Comment